Beat-and-glide swimming in larval zebrafish is an emergent property of their spinal network

University of Bristol (2005) J Physiol 567P, C106

Oral Communications: Beat-and-glide swimming in larval zebrafish is an emergent property of their spinal network

McLean, David L; Fetcho, Joseph R;

1. Neurobiology and Behavior, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA.

View other abstracts by:


Larval zebrafish (Danio rerio) primarily use a beat-and-glide swimming strategy. Here we explore which portion of the central nervous system produces this unique motor pattern. By chemically immobilizing intact zebrafish larvae (α-bungarotoxin, 12.5 μM) and recording from axial motor nerves at 1-3 different points with suction electrodes (Masino & Fetcho, 2005), we first confirmed that electrical stimulation (1 ms, 5-10 V) was eliciting swimming and not struggling, a more powerful rhythmic behavior. ‘Fictive’ swimming was characterized by cyclical bursts of motor activity (bursts 10.4 ± 0.7 ms long at frequencies of 27.2 ± 1.4 Hz, n = 11, mean ± standard error) that propagated from head to tail (0.7 ± 0.1 ms/segment, n = 5) and alternated from side to side (phase, 49.2 ± 0.1%, n = 9). As in unrestrained zebrafish larvae, we observed periodic bouts of swimming in immobilized larvae (bouts 210.7 ± 24.9 ms long every 4.1 ± 1.2 s, n = 10), which were more frequent after flashes of light (bouts 278.1 ± 21.7 ms long every 1.7 ± 0.2 s, n = 11). A brief electrical stimulus reliably evoked swimming, but persistent stimuli could elicit fictive struggling, which also alternated (phase, 45.7 ± 3.4%, n = 3), but was distinctly slower (13.5 ± 1.2 Hz, n = 3), with longer motor bursts (46.3 ± 3.3 ms, n = 3) in the opposite propagation direction (-6.3 ± 0.8 ms/segment, n = 3). Strychnine (10-15 μM) transformed swimming into prolonged bursts of periodic (140.2 ± 20.5 ms in duration every 1.6 ± 0.4 s, n = 3), bilaterally synchronous (phase, 97.9 ± 0.8%, n = 3) motor activity. This suggested that glycinergic signaling regulated the ‘beat’, but not the ‘glide’ mechanism. We next used paired patch-clamp recordings from spinal motor neurons (Drapeau et al., 1999) to assess the effects of spinalization on swimming. The spinal cord was transected between the 2nd-3rd muscle segments under anesthesia (0.2% MS-222). After 20-30 minutes recovery, we observed no periodic bouts of swimming, nor did light elicit any. Electrical stimuli generated spikes in motor neurons and, at higher stimulation levels (10-15 V), evoked extended episodes of swimming (bouts 5.4 ± 1.6 s long at frequencies of 21.3 ± 0.6 Hz, n = 6). Remarkably, once swimming was started, a periodic motor pattern appropriate to generate beat-and-glide swimming continued in the absence of further stimuli (bouts 319.4 ± 57.5 ms long every 3.2 ± 1.9 s, n = 5). While the rhythmic synaptic drive (20.9 ± 3.3 Hz, n = 5) was below threshold in primary motor neurons, it was often sufficient to cause secondary motor neurons to fire. Our data suggest that beat-and-glide swimming emerges from the rhythmic firing properties and connectivity of the spinal network. This locomotor network is likely activated and modulated by descending inputs.



Where applicable, experiments conform with Society ethical requirements.

Site search

Filter

Content Type